An MRI Study of the Relationship Between Human Neuroanatomy and Behavioral Ability
by
Paul Thomas Schoenemann
B.A. (University of California, Berkeley) 1986
M.A. (University of California, Berkeley) 1993
A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the
requirements for the degree of
Doctor of Philosophy
in
Anthropology
in the
GRADUATE DIVISION
of the
UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, BERKELEY
Committee in charge:
Professor Vincent M. Sarich
Professor William S.-Y. Wang
Professor F. Clark Howell
Professor Thomas F. Budinger
Spring 1997
Copyright 1997 by Paul Thomas Schoenemann
Hominid brain volume increased more than 3-fold in ~2.5 million years.
From an evolutionary perspective, natural selection for one or more
behavioral abilities is the most likely explanation. A large number of
studies have demonstrated that brain volume is related to performance on
standardized intelligence tests. The present study expands on this work
in three important ways. First, a sibling-pair design is used,
allowing for the calculation of independent within-family (WF) and
between-family (BF) correlations. Second, a broader set of cognitive
dimensions (specifically guided by an understanding of human evolution)
is investigated. Third, neuroanatomical components are quantified from
higher resolution 3D MR images (whole brain volume resolution of 1.3
mm3). The subject group consisted of 36 pairs of sisters.
The results for BF correlations between neuroanatomical and cognitive
variables are consistent with previous MRI-based studies, but WF
correlations are almost entirely negative or close to zero. The BF
correlation of brain volume to the first principal component of 11
cogntive tests (1st PC) is r=0.46 (p<0.01), but the corresponding WF
correlation is only r=-0.05 (NS). Social interest variables show a
similar pattern, while throwing ability and maturation rate are
unrelated to neuroanatomic variation both BF and WF.
This pattern is not easily explained by methodological or statistical
artifacts. This study also shows that family differences in
socio-economic status (SES) do not explain this pattern, because SES is
not correlated with brain volume. It is therefore highly unlikely that
the genetic correlation between neuroanatomy and behavior is zero.
Possible explanations are: 1) Behavioral variability is genetically
correlated only weakly with brain volume, precluding reliable detection
in relatively small sample sizes; 2) The types of tests used might not
assess behavior important for hominid brain evolution; 3) Within-family
environmental effects (e.g., sibling competition) may mask any
underlying genetic correlation. This is consistent with the finding
that individuals more than four years apart from their sister show a
correlation between brain volume and 1st PC of r=0.65 (p<0.023,
N=12). Those closer in age only show a correlation of r=0.15 (NS, N=60).
Furthermore, several cognitive tests are negatively correlated with WF
age differences.
Table of Contents
Chapter 2: Human Neuroanatomical Evolution
2.1.1.1 Causal Explanations of Brain to Body Size Scaling
2.1.1.2 Encephalization in Hominid Evolution
2.2 Grade Differences in Primate Brain Size
2.3 Other Aspects of Brain Evolution
2.3.1 Major Neuroanatomical Divisions
2.3.7 Functional Reorganization
2.3.7.1 Primary Visual Cortex and The Lunate Sulcus
Chapter 3: Evolutionary Explanations
3.2 Behavioral Benefits of Larger Brains
3.2.1 Brain size as an Indicator of the Complexity of Information Processing
3.2.2 Significant Behavioral Changes that Might Explain Hominid Brain Evolution
3.2.2.2 Social Ability as an Impetus to Brain Size Increase
3.2.2.3 Throwing, Hunting, and Spatial Ability
3.2.2.4 Diet and Human Evolution
3.2.2.5 Summary of Behavioral Changes Which Might Account for Hominid Brain Evolution
Chapter 4: Research into the Associations between Neuroanatomical Variation and Behavior
4.1 Hypotheses about the Functional Organization of the Brain
4.2 A Key Functional Area: The Frontal Lobes
4.3 The Meaning of Variation within Modern Human Populations
4.4 Attempts to Find Behavioral Correlates of Neuroanatomical Variation within Species
4.4.1 Evidence from Non-Human Species
4.4.2.2 Studies of Normal Individuals
4.4.2.3 Potentially Inconsistent Evidence
Chapter 5: Rationale of this Study
5.1 Limitations of Previous Studies
5.2 Contribution of the Present Research
5.2.1 Within-Family Correlations as a Test of Causality
5.2.1.1 Height/IQ and Myopia/IQ Associations: Test Cases
5.2.1.2 Brain size/IQ Studies of Between- versus Within-family Correlations
5.2.2 Expanding the Variety of Cognitive Dimensions Addressed
5.2.2.2 Additional linguistic tests
5.2.2.3
Throwing accuracy
5.2.2.4 Social dimensions
5.2.3 Expanding the Neuroanatomical Subdivisions Addressed
5.2.4 What is the Effect of Increasing Brain Size on Maturation-Rate?
5.2.5 Additional Variables of Interest
5.2.6 Increasing MRI Spatial Resolution
Chapter 6: Experimental Procedures
6.2.2 Inclusion/Exclusion Criteria
6.2.4 Consent Process and Documentation
6.2.5.2 Methods of Data Analysis
7.4 Between-family Correlations of Cognitive Tests with Neuroanatomical Features
7.5 Within-family Correlations of Cognitive Tests with Neuroanatomical Features
7.9.1 Reliability Differences and Restriction of Range
7.9.4 Consistency Across Subsets of Data
7.9.5 A Note About Age Correction
7.9.6 Summary of Methodological Issues
7.10 Mean Differences in Behavioral Variables Between Larger- and Smaller-Brained Siblings
8.1 Explanations for Lack of Within-Family Correlations
8.1.1 Between-Family Environmental Effects
8.1.2 Cross-Assortative Mating
8.1.3 Within-Family Environmental Effects
8.2 Implications for Hominid Brain Evolution
8.2.1 Other Cognitive Dimensions of Interest
8.2.2 How Strong Does the Association Need to Be?
8.4 Grey-White Matter Contrast and the Structure of the Brain
8.5 Possible Physiological Explanations of Brain Size Increases
Appendix A: Comparative and Fossil Data Tables
Table 1: Cranial Capacities and Dates for Selected Fossil Specimens (Modified Slightly from Falk 1987a)
Table 2: Brain sizes, body sizes, encephalization quotients, and 'extra brain mass' in 95 mammals (including 52 primates) and 1 reptile
Table 3: Estimated cranial capacity and body size of fossil hominids, extant pongids and Homo sapiens.
Appendix B: Questionnaire and Cognitive Test Details
Subject Questionnaire
Raven's Progressive Matrices (RAVEN) Instructions to Subjects
Simple Reaction Time test (SRT)
Instructions to Subjects
Object Identification Reaction Time test (OBJECT-ID)
Instructions to Subjects
Sequence of Test Trials
Sentence Verification test (SENTENCE-VERIF and SYNTAX)
Instructions to Subjects
Construction of the Sentences
Order of Appearance of Test Sentences:
Mental Rotation Reaction Time Test (MRT)
Instructions to Subjects
Wisconsin Card Sort Test - computerized adaptation (WCST)
Instructions Given to Subjects
Appendix C: Zero-order and Intraclass Correlations for the Variables Used in this Study
Zero-order Correlations
Among All 72 Individuals:
Between- and Within-family Zero-Order Correlations (N=36):
Intraclass Correlations:
Between-family Basic Statistics
Within-family Basic Statistics
Appendix D: MRI and Cognitive Testing Subject Consent Forms
MRI Consent Form
Cognitive Testing Consent Form
Appendix E: Line Drawings of the Human Brain
Lateral view
Superior view
Medial (mid-saggital) view
Inferior view
Figure 2.1 | Cranial capacity in fossil hominids over time |
Figure 2.2 | General relationship of body to brain size in mammals |
Figure 2.3 | Cranial capacity and body size in fossil hominids |
Figure 2.4 | Cerebral cortex volume and percent white matter in mammals |
Figure 2.5 | Gray and white matter cerebral volumes in mammals |
Figure 2.6 | Corpus callosum to brain size in 3 orders of mammals |
Figure 5.1 | Hypothetical causal links between SES, brain anatomy, and behavior |
Figure 5.2 | Theoretical independence of between and within family associations |
Figure 6.1 | Gaussian curve fitting on pixel intensity probability density functions |
Figure 7.1 | Association of BRAINVOL to RAVEN with family relationships indicated |
Figure 7.2 | Between family relationship of BRAINVOL to RAVEN |
Figure 7.3 | Relationship of proportion of variance to differences in size of correlation |
Figure 7.4 | Randomization study histograms |
Figure 8.1 | Comparison of older and younger sibling relationships between brain volume and general cognitive performance |
Figure 8.2 | Examples of negative within-family relationships between age and cognitive tests |
Figure E.1 | Landmarks and features of the lateral surface of the human brain |
Figure E.2 | Landmarks and features of the superior surface of the human brain |
Figure E.3 | Landmarks and features of the medial surface of the human brain |
Figure E.4 | Landmarks and features of the inferior surface of the human brain |
Table 2.1 |
Comparison of brain sizes between the great apes and humans, controlling for body weight |
Table 2.2 | Encephalization quotients of different neuroanatomical components based on non-human anthropoid primate trends with respect to body weight |
Table 2.3 | Encephalization quotients of different neuroanatomical components based on non-human anthropoid primate trends with respect to brain volume |
Table 6.1 |
Age data |
Table 6.2 | Schooling and socio-economic status |
Table 6.3 | Neuroanatomical variables |
Table 6.4 | Cognitive tests |
Table 6.5 | Handedness data |
Table 6.6 | Sociality indicators |
Table 6.7 | Menarche |
Table 6.8 | Throwing accuracy |
Table 6.9 | Amount of exercise |
Table 6.10 | Anthropometric data |
Table 6.11 | Test reliabilities |
Table 7.1 | Between family correlations among anthropometric variables |
Table 7.2 | Within family correlations among anthropometric variables |
Table 7.3 | Between family correlations among neuroanatomic variables |
Table 7.4 | Within family correlations among neuroanatomic variables |
Table 7.5 | Between family correlations of anthropometric and neuroanatomic variables |
Table 7.6 | Within family correlations of anthropometric and neuroanatomic variables |
Table 7.7 | Between family correlations among cognitive variables |
Table 7.8 | Within family correlations among cognitive variables |
Table 7.9 | Between family correlations of cognitive to neuroanatomic variables |
Table 7.10 | Between family correlations of neuroanatomic measures with cogntive 1st PC, and partial correlations with cogntive variables controlling for 1st PC |
Table 7.11 | Within family correlations of cognitive to neuroanatomic |
Table 7.12 | Within family correlations of neuroanatomic measures with cogntive 1st PC, and partial correlations with cogntive variables controlling for 1st PC |
Table 7.13 | Between family correlations of neuroanatomic to sociality measures |
Table 7.14 | Within family correlations of neuroanatomic to sociality measures |
Table 7.15 | Between family correlations of sociality with cognitive variables |
Table 7.16 | Within family correlations of sociality with cognitive variables |
Table 7.17 | Between and within family correlations of MENARCHE with neuroanatomic variables |
Table 7.18 | Between and within family correlations of THROW (throwing accuracy) with neuroanatomic variables |
Table 7.19 | Estimated effect of range-restriction and reliability differences on the largest associations with BRAINVOL found in this study |
Table 7.20 | Average within-family differences for the cognitive variables in this study |
Table 7.21 | Average within-family differences for the First PC, and the other cognitive variables controlling for First PC |
Table 7.22 | Average within-family differences for MENARCHE, THROW, and sociality measures |
Table 7.23 | Average within-family differences for the cognitive variables in this study grouped by within-family BRAINVOL differences |
Table 7.24 | Average within-family differences for the First PC and the other cognitive variables controlling for First PC grouped by within-family BRAINVOL differences |
Table 7.25 | Average within-family differences for MENARCHE, THROW, and sociality variables grouped by within-family BRAINVOL differences |
Table 7.26 | Between and within family associations between SYNTAX and six possible correlates of past exposure to esoteric syntax |
Table 8.1 | Between family correlations of SES with neuroanatomic variables |
Table 8.2 | Within-family correlations of cognitive to neuroanatomic variables for sibling pairs that differ by less than four years in age (N=30) |
Table 8.3 | Within-family correlations of cognitive to neuroanatomic variables for sibling pairs that differ by more than four years in age (N=6) |
Table 8.4 | Average within-family differences for the cognitive variables grouped by within-family age differences |
Table 8.5 | Average within-family differences for the First PC and the other cognitive variables controlling for First PC, grouped by within-family age differences |
Table 8.6 | Average within-family differences for MENARCHE, THROW, and sociality variables grouped by within-family age differences |
Table 8.7 | Within-Family Correlations Between Cognitive Tests and Age
at Testing |
Copyright 1997 by Paul Thomas Schoenemann